False confessions are not a new concept in America, and
there have been hundreds of examples of false confessions. In Massachusetts in
1692 many colonists confessed to being witches in Salem. The trials resulted in
at least nineteen executions before they were eventually stopped (Agar, 1999).
Further along in American history, the Lindbergh kidnapping and murder became a
public media skeptical in 1932. During that time, over 200 innocent people came
forward and confessed to the kidnapping and murder of Charles Lindbergh’s child
(Agar, 1999). More currently with the advancement of DNA techniques, confessed
sentenced criminals are being released from prison for crimes they never
committed. Why would anyone ever confess to a crime, especially a serious one,
when they never committed the crime? Current criminal justice professionals,
psychologists, and sociologists argue that the suspect’s mental and
psychological condition may affect many false confessions. Suspect
psychological characteristics, type of crime, and contextual characteristics of
the interrogations can affect a suspect’s tendency to confess (Gudjonsson,
2003).
Gudjonsson (2003) discussed different psychological
variables that effect confessions. These include current mental status,
intellectual functioning, reading ability, interrogative suggestibility, state
and trait anxiety, and the understanding of rights by questioned suspects
(Gudjonsson, 2003). For these variables to be properly examined, investigators
conducting a criminal interview must determine if the suspect is fit to be
interviewed. Investigators must assess the cognition and intellect of the
suspect to be best of their abilities. The investigators tasked with
interviewing a suspect should be aware of any psychiatric disorder that will
usually lead to unfitness for an interview and psychiatric disorders that might
indicate a significant risk of unreliability (Rix, 1997). Another mental status that is currently being studied are individuals with ADHD. A study by Gudjonsson, Sigurdsson, Bragason, Newton, and Elinarsson (2008) studied 90 Icelandic prisoners who were currently serving prison sentences and diagnosed with ADHD. The study determined reasons why ADHD prisoners made false confessions for previous crimes that they did not commit. The reasons were: nine (43%) wanted to leave the police station or to avoid custody and six (29%) were protecting someone else (Gudjonsson, Sigurdsson, Bragason, Newton, and Elinarsson 2008). This study concluded that ADHD suspects are particularly vulnerable during police questioning and detention because of their restlessness, poor concentration, and impulsiveness (Gudjonsson, Sigurdsson, Bragason, Newton, and Elinarsson 2008). The findings of this study are not unexpected, as anyone with any experience with ADHD can understand. Besides certain mental status conditions another issue in confession is the understanding of legal rights during questioning and detention.
Understanding legal rights is another major psychological variable in confessions. It is estimated that 695,000 mentally disordered offenders are arrested and Mirandized annually in the Unites States (Rogers, Harrison, Hazelwood, & Sewell, 2007). Miranda v. Arizona (1966) established procedural safeguards to protect suspects in interviews to avoid making self-incriminating statements and to have an attorney present during questioning. The suspect must make a confession “knowingly, intelligently, and voluntarily” (Miranda v. Arizona, 1966). A study conducted by O’Connell, Garmoe, and Goldstein (2005) determined that individuals with lower IQ scores had a higher tendency to be tricked by leading questions. The same study determined that individuals with mild mental retardation demonstrated difficultly understanding Miranda rights. If a suspect cannot understand their legal rights, then their rights can be violated—making any voluntary confession not admissible.
Representation should always be present and if applicable a psychologist who specialized in the disorder should also be present to explain the suspect’s rights. In situations where the suspect suffers from ADHD, investigators should be trained in how to interview individuals with the condition and every effort should be made to make the interview short and brief. Personal statements from individuals affected by these conditions should not be discredited, since they may be telling the truth in a voluntary confession.
References
Agar, J. (1999). “The Admissibility of False Confession Expert Testimony”. The Army Lawyer; Aug. pp. 26-43.
Gudjonsson, G. (2003). The Psychology of Interrogations and Confessions. Wiley, England, 2003.
Gudjonsson, G., Sigurdsson, J., Bragason, O., Newton, A., & Elinarsson, E. (2008).
“Interrogative suggestibility, compliance and false confessions among prisoners and their relationship with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms”. Psychological Medicine (2008), 38, pp. 1037-1044.
Miranda v. Arizona, 384 U.S. 436 (1966).
O’Connell, M., Garmoe, W., & Goldstein, N. (2005). “Miranda Comprehension in Adults with Mental Retardation and the Effects of Feedback Style on Suggestibility”. Law and Human Behavior, Vol. 29, No. 3, June 2005.
Rix, K. (1997). “Fit to be interviewed by the police?”. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, vol. 3, pp. 33-40.
Rogers, R., Harrison, K., Hazelwood, L., & Sewell, K. (2007). “Knowing and Intelligent: A Study of Miranda Warnings in Mentally Disordered Defendants”. Law of Human Behavior, Vol. 31: pp. 401-418.
Thanks very much for your great in sequence . And information full description . I feel it is Sus a topic that many kinds of people features many problems. thanks for this. . I think Southern California Neuropsychology Group is extremely nice.
ReplyDeleteIt’s amazing in support of me to truly have a web site that is valuable meant for my knowledge.
ReplyDeletehave a peek at this web-site