In a study conducted by Drizin and Leo (2004), a study was conducted of 125 cases of proven false confessions that occurred between 1971 and 2002. One-third of the cases examined consisted of juvenile offenders under the age of 17 (Drizin & Leo, 2004). Interesting, over 90% of the cases involved major charges such as murder or rape (Drizin & Leo, 2004). Other empirical studies have also concluded that juveniles are more likely to falsely incriminate themselves and confess during an interrogation than older persons and adults. In a study of exonerations conducted by Gross, Jacoby, Matheson, Montgomery, & Patil (2005) found that 33 of 340 individuals who were exonerated between 1989 and 2003 were under the age of 18 at the time of the crimes that they were convicted. Fourteen of those thirty-three that were exonerated admitted that they had falsely confessed- 42% compared to 13% of older exonerees (Gross, Jacoby, Matheson, Montgomery, & Patil, 2005). Out of the youngest of these juveniles that were exonerated, those aged twelve to fifteen-69% confessed to homicides (and one rape) that they did not commit (Gross, Jacoby, Matheson, Montgomery, & Patil, 2005).
Many argue that juveniles are more vulnerable to coercion and suggestion than their adult counterparts. Current studies have determined that younger children are more suggestible than older children in terms of giving in to both leading questions and interrogative pressure (Gudjonsson, 2003). Studies have determined that adolescents are more responsive to negative feedback during an interrogation than adults. In a study conducted by Warren, Hulse-Trotter, & Tubbs (1991), younger children (7 years of age) yielded more to misleading questions and shifted their answers more after negative feedback than children who were older and adults (Gujonsson, 2003). The same study determined that 7 year olds and 12 years olds both shifted their answers significantly more after negative feedback than did adults in the same study (Gujonsson, 2003). Many theorize why juveniles are more vulnerable than their adult counterparts is because of the social expectations of obedience to authority and the juvenile’s actual perceived social status. With a perceived lower social status, a juvenile may be more likely to speak indirectly with authority figures to avoid provoking conflict (Feld, 2006).
The perceived increase in false confessions can be attributed to a number of different reasons in the criminal justice field. But the alarming studies of false confessions among juveniles can not be ignored. Juveniles, who are making up more and more of the criminal offenders in the criminal justice field, have been shown to be more suggestible and more likely to be coerced than their adult counterparts. This increase in questioning can lead to a false confession by a juvenile confronted by an authoritative figure with leading questions. Current investigators should be aware of the suggestibility of juvenile offenders and their desire to please those in an authoritive role asking misleading questions. Investigators should constantly remember that the main goal of justice is to arrest the guilty party and protect the innocent.
REFERENCES
Drizin, S. & Leo, R. (2004). The problem of false confessions in the post-DNA world. North Carolina Law Review, 82, 891-1007.
Feld, B. (2006). Police Interrogation of Juveniles: An Empirical Study of Policy and Practice. Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 97, 219-316.
Gross, S., Jacoby, K., Matheson, D., Montgomery, N., & Patil, S. (2005). Exonerations in the Unite States, 1989 through 2003. Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 95, 523-560.
Gudjonsson, G. (2003). The Psychology of Interrogations and Confessions. West Sussex, England: Wiley.
Leo, R. & Ofshe, R. (1998). The consequences of false confessions: Deprivations of liberty and miscarriages of justice in the age of psychological interrogation. Journal of Criminal Law & Criminology, 88, 429-496.
Scheck, B., Neufeld, P. & Dwyer, J. (2000). Actual Innocence. Garden City, NJ: Doubleday.